Jelly and Bean

Philosophy

In written language, our thoughts and our speech are represented by visual symbols to convey meaning. In an alphabetic language like English, the 'visual symbols' are letters. Letters are combined into words which have meaning for us. Each word is separated by a space from the next word. In this way written text is displayed in groups of letters that have meaning for us. We learn how to read these words.

In the 17th century, the spellings of English words were 'set in stone' when the first dictionaries were written. These spellings have not changed over time. It is because they have not changed over the years, that we can read books written in previous centuries.

In some languages each letter corresponds to a single sound. Learning to read and write is fairly easy in these languages, but the English language has only 26 letters in its alphabet and at least 44 different sounds. There is no single correspondence between each letter and each sound.

Sometimes a combination of letters corresponds to a single sound e.g. 'ch' as in 'chip'. Sometimes the same letters correspond to a different sound, e.g. 'ch' in 'chemist' or 'ch' as in 'chef'.

Sometimes a single letter can represent more than one sound, e.g. 'a' in 'cat, father, was, water, baby'. Each 'a' corresponds to a different sound. The letter 'y' is pronounced differently in 'mummy, my, yet, gym' as well as being part of vowel graphemes in 'ay (day), ey (grey), oy (toy), ey (donkey)'. This makes learning to read and write in English very difficult. Sometimes we need to know the meaning of a word before we can decide how to pronounce it, e.g. 'row, row; wound, wound; wind, wind; beak, bread, break'

We can learn all the different pronunciations of the letters and letter combinations, and still this will not tell us to how to choose the correct pronunciation of a word without knowing what it means. We need to relate the spelling of the word to something else we already know, i.e. what the word means.

Let's consider letter symbols too. If we think of the first letter of the alphabet, 'a' or 'A, we see straight away that it has two written symbols. (In fact 'a' has more than two symbols because children do not write the small letter 'a' in this particular type-font. They use the shape of 'a' as that in the Sassoon Primary Infant, Century Gothic or Comic Sans fonts.)

The first symbol is lower case on the keyboard, or a 'small' letter. The second symbol is upper case on the keyboard, or a 'capital letter'. A capital letter is used at the beginning of a sentence or the beginning of a name, and its corresponding sound is the same as that of the small letter. We would pronounce 'cat, baby, father,' exactly the same if they were written in capital letters 'CAT, BABY, FATHER' or a mixture of letters 'Cat, Baby, Father'.

So the capital letters do not correspond to different sounds and they do not add to our 26 letters in the alphabet.

If we pronounce the words 'car' and 'father', 'down' and 'about', 'form' and 'pawn', 'royal' and 'boil', 'girl' and 'curl', 'hair' and 'stare', 'year' and 'deer', we can hear the same sound for the vowels in each pair of words, but the spellings are different.

How do we learn which spelling goes with which sound in which word? We can only do that if we know the meaning of the word and we have this extra information to help us. The sound of the word alone will not always tell us how to spell it. We need to visually memorise it also.

If we say the alphabet aloud, we say 'ay, bee, see, dee, ee, eff, gee, aich, I, jay, kay, ell, em, en, oh, pea, queue, are, ess, tee, you, vee, double-you, ex, why, zed'. We notice that these are not the same as the sounds of the letters of the alphabet. They are the names of the letters of the alphabet.

When someone asks us to spell the word 'dog', we will say 'dee, oh, gee'. These letter names are not the same as the sounds in the word 'dog'. Children have to learn these letter names too.

Also, some of the sounds corresponding to the consonant letters of the alphabet are impossible to articulate without an audible sound of exhaled air, e.g. 'b, d, p, t'. 

(There are some continuant consonants that can be easily pronounced e.g. 'sssss' (the hiss of a snake) or 'shhhh' (be quiet) or 'mmmmmm' (yummy) or 'ffffff' and 'rrrrrr'.)

Human speech is a continuous stream of ever changing sound and it is very difficult to segment (split up). It is not made up of separate sounds which we are consciously aware of when we are speaking and listening. No one needs to be aware of separate sounds in speech until they are learning to read and write. In fact we actually speak at too fast a rate to be able to pick out the sounds in speech in normal conversations. It is only on reflection and with afterthought that we become aware of them. This is why children have to be taught about them if they are going to be able to relate the letters they see in words to the sounds they hear in words.

These sound-symbol relationships are called phoneme/grapheme correspondences or grapheme/phoneme correspondences. They are sometimes referred to as GPCs. Children have to be taught them if they are learning to read by a phonic method.

The two main methods of teaching phonics are analytic phonics and synthetic phonics. 

In analytic phonics, children are shown whole written words first. They are then shown how the letters correspond to the sounds in the words.

In synthetic phonics, children are taught sounds first. Then they are taught the corresponding letters. When they know a few corresponding letters and sounds, they are taught how to blend the sounds into words. They 'synthesise' the sounds to arrive at the word.

Our books may be used to support either of these methods of teaching. They are simply resources. They may be used with any method of teaching because we start with the simplest written words from which letter/sounds relationships can easily be seen and heard, and we move on to more complicated written words gradually.

Our first books have only the simplest CVC (consonant-vowel-consonant) words which children understand, e.g. 'cat, dog, hat, box'. Children know what these words mean from their use in spoken language and picture books. Initially, each word is introduced in our early books with a picture to ensure that the children know the meaning of the word. When each word is split up into the sounds corresponding to the letters, children are able to see and hear the letter/sound correspondences.

In our first books these words are joined by  'a, on, in, and' to link them into phrases. There are pictures to ensure that children understand the meanings of the joining words 'in, a, on, and'. These words may then be split up into the sounds of the letters so that children learn the letter/sound correspondences.

We introduce the word 'the' early in the books, because it is impossible to write anything meaningful without it. Children very quickly learn how to pronounce it as a whole word and they soon recognise it when they see it.

The vocabulary used in our books means that we introduce the hundred most frequently used words in English at the same time as we introduce the main grapheme/phoneme correspondences and we do this gradually. Children are not overwhelmed by too many new ideas at once. They gain confidence when reading successfully, and this gives them the motivation to learn more.

Our aim is to make reading enjoyable for children and to give them pleasurable experiences which they want to repeat. Then, as their vocabulary grows, they have the necessary skills to read the new words they meet in the wider school curriculum.

Delivery Information
Inspection copies
Teaching guides
Free writing resources
The latest news from Jelly and Bean

Is Sheep a CVC word?

read more
The latest news from Jelly and Bean

Are phonemes the basic units of speech?

read more
The latest news from Jelly and Bean

100 Words

read more